Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management, Volume 7, Issue 2, 2012
Measuring burnout and work engagement: Factor structure,
invariance, and latent mean differences across Greece and the
Netherlands
Despoina Xanthopoulou
School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki
Old Building, GR-54124, Thessaloniki, Greece
Telephone: +30 2310 997216
Email: dxanthopoulou@psy.auth.gr
Arnold B. Bakker
Department of Work & Organizational Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam
Institute of Psychology
PO BOX 1738, 3000 DR, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Telephone: + 31 10 4088853
Email: bakker@fsw.eur.nl
Aristotelis Kantas
Department of Primary Education, University of Patras
Archimedes str., Building 7
Telephone: + 30 2610 996152
Email: akantas@upatras.gr
Evangelia Demerouti
Department of Industrial Engineering & Innovation Sciences, Section of Human Performance Management,
Eindhoven University of Technology
PO BOX 513, 5600 MB, Eindhoven, The Netherlands
Telephone: + 31 40 2475669
Email: e.demerouti@tue.nl
Abstract
This study examines the factor structure and invariance of the instruments measuring burnout (Maslach Burnout
Inventory - General Survey / MBI-GS) and work engagement (Utrecht Work Engagement Scale / UWES) in a
sample of Dutch (N = 162) and Greek (N = 206) employees. Confirmatory factor analyses in both samples
supported the superiority of the proposed three-factor structure of both the MBI-GS (exhaustion, cynicism, and
reduced professional efficacy) and the UWES (vigor, dedication, and absorption). Alternative two-factor and
one-factor models did not show a better fit to the data. In addition, results of multigroup analyses partly
supported the invariance of the three-factor model of the MBI-GS, and fully supported the invariance of the
three-factor model of the UWES across the two national samples. These results suggest that the MBI-GS and the
UWES are not only valid instruments for testing burnout and engagement but also allow comparisons across
countries.
Keywords: burnout, cross-national study, factor structure, invariance, latent mean differences, work
engagement
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
41
1 INTRODUCTION
Organizational psychologists and managers are interested in burnout because it is a significant correlate of
employee impaired health, and reduced performance (Bakker, Van Emmerik, & Van Riet, 2008; Shirom, 2003).
Over the past decade, researchers have shifted the attention from employee malfunctioning to optimal
functioning. This latter development reflects the trend towards a ‘positive psychology’ that focuses on human
strengths rather than on weaknesses (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Luthans (2002) underlined the need
for positive organizational behavior research, defined as ‘the study and application of positively oriented human
resource strengths and psychological capacities that can be measured, developed, and effectively managed for
performance improvement in today’s workplace (p. 59). In this context, researchers and managers became
interested in understanding not only what makes employees burned out, but also what makes them energetic and
enthusiastic about their work (Bakker, Schaufeli, Leiter, & Taris, 2008). This is because engaged employees
enjoy better health, are more proactive and perform better (for a review see Bakker, Demerouti, &
Xanthopoulou, 2012), while engagement relates positively to financial turnover (Xanthopoulou, Bakker,
Demerouti, & Schaufeli, 2009) and client loyalty (Salanova, Agut, & Peiró, 2005). Thus, valid instruments to
estimate employee burnout and engagement are necessary both for researchers and managers.
Together with this positive trend, the need for cross-cultural studies in organizational psychology research
has become more evident. However, there are methodological difficulties that cross-cultural researchers
frequently encounter. One major problem occurs when cross-cultural comparisons are made between responses
to questionnaires, which have not been validated psychometrically across cultures (Smith, Fisher, & Sale, 2001).
Nevertheless, the need for valid psychometric measures is better understood than in the past. Several studies
have compared levels of burnout and work engagement across different countries. However, apart from some
exceptions (Mäkikangas, tinen, Kinnunen, & Pekkonen, 2011; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010; Schaufeli,
Salanova, González-Romá, & Bakker, 2002b; Schaufeli & Van Dierendonck, 1993), the instruments used were
simply translated and their psychometric properties have been taken for granted. Furthermore, none of the
previous studies estimated whether the relations between the underlying constructs and the specific items are
invariant across the national groups (i.e. scalar invariance). This issue raises concerns about the validity of the
instruments, when used for comparisons across different settings.
The central aim of the present study is to examine the factor structure and invariance of two instruments
that are broadly used to measure burnout (Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey; MBI-GS; Schaufeli,
Leiter, Maslach & Jackson, 1996) and work engagement (Utrecht Work Engagement Scale; UWES; Schaufeli et
al., 2002b) across Greece and The Netherlands. This would allow valid estimations and comparisons of the
levels of burnout and engagement among employees from both national groups. The advantage of this study as
compared to previous work (for a review see, Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010; Schaufeli et al., 2002b) is that we do
not only test the configural and metric invariance of these instruments across the two national samples, but also
their scalar invariance. Only when scalar invariance is met, we are sure that we measure the same construct and
thus, it is possible to compare means across samples with generalizable results (Vandenberg & Lance, 2000).
Even though Greece and The Netherlands share the same European background, working condition surveys
systematically show that Dutch employees enjoy more favourable working conditions than Greek employees
(Eurofound, 2007; 2012). Therefore, we expect that Greek and Dutch employees will exhibit different levels of
burnout and work engagement. We compare employees from Greece and the Netherlands because the UWES
(Schaufeli & Bakker, 2003) has been initially developed in the Dutch language, while most empirical evidence
on the validity of the MBI-GS (Schaufeli et al., 1996) is based on Dutch employees. Thus, it makes sense to
compare the Greek versions of these instruments with the original and/or validated ones.
2 LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES
2.1 Burnout and Engagement
Over the course of their careers, many employees experience a great deal of job strain that may include
exhaustion, disengagement and health complaints, as a result of a demanding and emotionally charged work
environment (Schaufeli & Greenglass, 2001). These experiences may lead to poor performance, high levels of
absenteeism, and personnel turnover. For more than two decades, clinical observations have been made and
empirical research has been conducted on this phenomenon, which has been labelled “burnout”. Burnout is
defined as a syndrome of exhaustion, cynicism towards work and reduced professional efficacy, occurring
among individuals in relation to their work (Maslach, Jackson & Leiter, 1996). According to Schaufeli et al.
(1996), exhaustion -the central component of burnout- refers to energy depletion and is characterized by mental,
emotional and physical tiredness. Cynicism refers to the development of negative attitudes towards one’s job or
the recipients of one’s work an extreme and therefore dysfunctional kind of detachment and loss of concern.
Finally, lack of professional efficacy is the tendency to evaluate one’s work negatively. Such an evaluation is
often accompanied by feelings of insufficiency, self-doubt and poor self-esteem (Maslach, 1993).
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
42
Instead of looking exclusively at the negative side of employee well-being, burnout research also focuses
on the positive side. Maslach and Leiter (1997; 2008) rephrased burnout as an erosion of engagement with the
job. Accordingly, engagement is characterized by energy, involvement and efficacy - the direct opposites of the
three burnout dimensions. Schaufeli and Bakker (2001) have taken a different approach to the concept of work
engagement, according to which engagement is defined and operationalized in its own right. They defined work
engagement as a positive, affective-motivational state of fulfilment in employees that is characterized by vigor,
dedication and absorption. Vigor refers to high levels of energy and resilience, the willingness to invest effort in
one’s job, the ability to not be easily fatigued and to be persistent in the face of difficulties. Dedication refers to
a strong involvement in one’s work, accompanied by feelings of enthusiasm and significance and by a sense of
pride and inspiration. Finally, absorption refers to a pleasant state of total immersion in one’s work, which is
characterized by time passing quickly and being unable to detach oneself from the job (Schaufeli & Bakker,
2010; Schaufeli, Martínez, Marques-Pinto, Salanova, & Bakker, 2002a).
2.2 The Measurement of Burnout and Engagement
Originally, burnout was conceived as a three-dimensional syndrome of emotional exhaustion,
depersonalisation and reduced personal accomplishment that occurs in individuals who do people work (i.e.,
education, social services or health care), and was measured with the Maslach Burnout Inventory-Human
Services Survey (MBI-HSS; Maslach & Jackson, 1981). However, research has shown that burnout is not
restricted to individuals who work with people but the syndrome can also be found outside the context of human
services (Bakker, Demerouti & Schaufeli, 2002; Leiter & Schaufeli, 1996). Therefore, the concept of burnout
and its measurement were broadened to include all employees, irrespective of their occupation. As a result, the
original version of the MBI was adapted for use outside the human services and transformed into the MBI-GS
(Schaufeli et al., 1996).
The three dimensions of the MBI-GS (Schaufeli et al., 1996) parallel those of the original MBI, in the sense
that they are more generic and do not refer to individuals as the object of one’s work. For example, exhaustion is
measured by items that do not include other people as the source of the fatigue (e.g. “I feel tired when I get up in
the morning and I have to face another day on the job”). The same applies for cynicism and (reduced)
professional efficacy. In particular, the items that measure cynicism reflect a distance towards work in general
and not only regarding working with other people (e.g. “I doubt the significance of my work”). Finally, the
items that measure professional efficacy focus both on social and non-social occupational accomplishments (e.g.
“I have accomplished many worthwhile things in this job”).
Psychometric research with the MBI-GS has shown that the three-factor structure is invariant across
occupations (Bakker et al., 2002; Leiter & Schaufeli, 1996; Mäkikangas et al., 2011; Taris, Schreurs, &
Schaufeli, 1999), and across samples from Finland, Norway, and The Netherlands (Schutte, Toppinnen, Kalimo,
& Schaufeli, 2000). Only one study examined the psychometric properties of the Greek version of the MBI-GS
(Demerouti, Bakker, Vardakou & Kantas, 2003). Results of this study provided evidence for the three-factor
structure of the MBI-GS across various occupations in Greece. However, analyses showed that there are
problems with some items, mostly of the cynicism scale. Therefore, the first objective of the present study is to
re-examine the factorial validity of the MBI-GS across a sample of Greek and Dutch employees. The following
hypothesis is formulated:
Hypothesis 1: The three-factor structure of the MBI-GS (exhaustion, cynicism, and reduced professional
efficacy) will be the same in Greece and the Netherlands.
As mentioned earlier, research on work engagement was actually developed through the research on
burnout (Maslach et al., 2001; Maslach & Leiter, 1997, 2008). First, Maslach and Leiter (1997) conceptualized
engagement as the opposite pole of burnout and assumed that engagement is characterized by energy,
involvement and efficacy, which are the exact opposites of the three burnout dimensions: exhaustion, cynicism,
and reduced professional efficacy. Engaged employees are strongly connected to their work tasks and believe
that they are effective in dealing with their job demands. Based on this approach, engagement is measured by
the opposite pattern of scores on the three MBI-GS dimensions (Maslach et al., 2001). This means that low
scores on exhaustion and cynicism, and high scores on professional efficacy are indicative of engagement.
Schaufeli and Bakker (2001; Schaufeli et al., 2002a; 2002b) followed a different approach in defining work
engagement. They argued that Maslach and Leiter’s (1997) approach precludes an investigation of the
relationship between burnout and engagement, since both concepts are considered to be the opposite poles of a
continuum that is assessed with one instrument (the MBI-GS). Engagement cannot be measured by the opposite
profile of the MBI-GS, because, even though in conceptual terms it is the positive antithesis of burnout, the
structure and the measurement of both concepts are different. Importantly, burnout and engagement do not share
the same antecedents and are explained by different psychological mechanisms (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010).
Burnout is mainly determined by the demanding aspects of the job (e.g. workload, physical demands), while
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
43
work engagement is mainly determined by the availability of resources in the work environment (e.g. autonomy
or support; for a meta-analysis, see Halbesleben, 2010). This argumentation explains why engagement cannot be
measured with the opposite scores of burnout, and should be measured on its own right. Thus, Schaufeli and
Bakker (2001; 2010) proposed that burnout and engagement should be conceived as two distinct concepts that
should be measured independently with different instruments. In this context, the Utrecht Work Engagement
Scale (UWES) has been developed that measures the three underlying dimensions of engagement (i.e. vigor,
dedication, and absorption).
In a recent study by Demerouti, Mostert, and Bakker (2010), where the MBI-GS and the UWES were
examined, results showed that the cynicism dimension of burnout and the dedication dimension of engagement
can be considered as the opposite ends of the same attitude dimension called “identification” (see also
González-Romá, Schaufeli, Bakker, & Lloret, 2006). However, the same cannot be said for the exhaustion and
vigor dimensions that do not seem to represent the different ends of the “energy” continuum, despite being
highly related. These conclusions were further supported by the results that exhaustion associated mainly to
work pressure (i.e. a typical job demand), while vigor to job autonomy (i.e. a typical job resource). All in all,
empirical evidence suggests that burnout (as measured with the MBI-GS) and engagement (as measured with
the UWES) seem to be related but independent constructs, which further supports the use of the UWES for the
measurement of work engagement.
The UWES initially included fifteen items (a later version includes seventeen items, while a shorter version
includes only nine items; Schaufeli, Bakker, & Salanova, 2006), which are assumed to assess the three
underlying dimensions of engagement: vigor, dedication and absorption. Results based on Dutch (Schaufeli &
Bakker, 2004; Schaufeli et al., 2002a) and Spanish samples of employees and university students (Schaufeli et
al., 2002b) showed that the fit of the hypothesized three-factor model to the data was superior to alternative
models (see also Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010). Schaufeli et al. (2006) supported the three-factor structure of the
UWES across different countries (e.g. Australia, Canada, Belgium, South Africa, France, and Finland) and
across occupations. Also, Nerstad, Richardsen, and Martinussen, (2010) supported the three-factor structure
across occupational groups in Norway. In the present study, we follow Schaufeli et al.’s (2002a; 2002b)
approach on engagement, and we use the original fifteen-item UWES. The second objective of our study is to
test the factorial validity of the UWES across Greece and The Netherlands. This is interesting because there is
no evidence so far on the validity of the scale in a Greek sample. On the basis of previous findings (Schaufeli et
al., 2002a), we formulate the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 2: The three-factor structure of the UWES (vigor, dedication, and absorption) will be the same
in Greece and the Netherlands.
2.3. A Cross-national Comparison of Burnout and Engagement
The present study also concerns comparisons between Greek and Dutch employees regarding their levels of
burnout and engagement. The interesting point in comparing Greece and The Netherlands is that both countries
are very different in terms of their work characteristics and the opportunities provided. The most recent
European survey on working conditions shows that the work environment of Greek employees is significantly
more demanding and less resourceful than that of Dutch employees (Eurofound, 2012). More specifically, Greek
employees work longer hours than Dutch, they are at a higher level of risk exposure, they perceive lower levels
of autonomy at work, lower task rotation, and they receive significantly less training. Interestingly, profit-
sharing schemes (where employees receive part of the profits generated by the company they work for) is a
common practice in the Netherlands, but almost negligible in Greece. Also, Greek employees are among the
first in Europe to report that their work affects their health in a negative way. These differences are important
because theoretical models explaining burnout and engagement suggest that demanding work environments (i.e.
characterized by high workload, high risks, and cognitive demands) are responsible for employee burnout, while
resourceful work environments (i.e. characterized by high levels of autonomy, support, employee-friendly
practices) relate to employee engagement (Demerouti, Bakker, Schaufeli, & Nachreiner, 2001).
Furthermore, the level of unemployment has been historically significantly higher in Greece than in The
Netherlands (Eurofound, 2007; 2012). This suggests that problems regarding job insecurity are more prevalent
in Greece. Demerouti et al. (2003) used the Dutch norm scores to classify the Greek respondents of their study
in low, medium, high and very high levels on the three MBI-GS dimensions and they found that one third to half
of the Greek participants was experiencing high or very high levels of burnout. Finally, Dutch were found to be
amongst the most engaged employees in Europe (Taipale, Selander, Anttila, & tti, 2011), while studies
outside the domain of occupational health psychology have shown that Dutch individuals are happier than
Greeks (Veenhoven, 2000). These results suggest that Greek employees are more likely to be burned out and
less likely to be engaged than Dutch employees:
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
44
Hypothesis 3: Greek employees will report higher levels of burnout and lower levels of work engagement
than Dutch employees.
3 METHOD
3.1 Samples and Procedure
Sample 1 concerned a heterogeneous group of 206 Greek employees (101 men, 49%; 105 women, 51%)
from a large public enterprise (49%; health care employees, administration and technical staff) and from private
profit organizations (i.e. two banks, a department store, a sports union and a travel agency; 51%), where
employees were mainly working in customer-service jobs (e.g., sales). The majority of participants (43.2%)
were between thirty and forty years of age and held a bachelor degree (44.7%). Their mean organizational
tenure was 7.8 years (SD = 5.7), and the response rate was 74%.
Sample 2 included 162 Dutch employees (79 men, 49%; 83 women, 51%). The sample was also
heterogeneous since the questionnaire was administered to both public servants (33%; teachers and nurses), and
employees working in the private sector [therapists (28%), managers (23%), technicians (4%) and
administrators (5%)]. The remaining of the participants (7%) did not report their occupation. The mean age of
participants was 44 years (SD = 8.7). The majority of the participants (53%) held a bachelor degree. Their mean
organizational tenure was 10.9 years (SD = 8.3), and the response rate was 79%.
In both countries, researchers informed the managers or CEOs of the organizations that were approached
about the purposes of the study. After receiving their consent, employees from these organizations were invited
to participate in a study about ‘work-related well-being’. Participants of both samples were approached directly
by the researchers and were asked to fill in a questionnaire about their work life. All contacts between
researchers and participants were face-to-face. In all cases, employees were reassured that participation was
anonymous and that data would be treated confidentially.
3.2 Measures
Burnout was measured with the Dutch (Schutte et al., 2000) and Greek (Demerouti et al., 2003) version of
the MBI-GS (Schaufeli et al., 1996). The MBI-GS includes 16 items that assess the three dimensions of burnout.
Exhaustion is measured with five items, such as: “I feel emotionally drained from my work”. Cynicism is also
assessed with five items, including: “I have become less enthusiastic about my work”. Finally, professional
efficacy is assessed with six items, for example: “I can effectively solve the problems that arise in my work”. All
items are scored on a seven-point scale, ranging from (0) “never” to (6) “every day”. High scores on exhaustion
and cynicism and a low score on professional efficacy indicate burnout. Previous studies that tested the factorial
validity of the MBI-GS (Demerouti et al., 2003; Schutte et al., 2000) have shown that all items load significantly
on the intended factors, save one exception. One item (item 4; “I just want to do my job and not be bothered”) of
the cynicism sub-scale proved to be ambivalent, and did not load significantly on the cynicism factor. For that
reason, as suggested by Schutte et al. (2000), we excluded this item from the following analyses.
Work Engagement was assessed with the Dutch (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2003) and Greek 15-item version of
the UWES. The UWES was translated in Greek from the Dutch original and was checked for accuracy with the
method of back-translation. The UWES items reflect three underlying dimensions, which were measured with
five items each: Vigor (e.g. “At my work, I feel bursting with energy”), Dedication (e.g. “am enthusiastic about
my job”) and Absorption (e.g. “I get carried away when I am working”). All items are scored on a seven-point
scale, ranging from (0) “never” to (6) “every day”. High scores on all three dimensions indicate work
engagement.
3.3 Strategy of Analysis
In order to test Hypotheses 1 and 2, competing models were examined with confirmatory factor analyses
(CFAs; Vandenberg & Lance, 2000), using the AMOS 19.0 software package. The three burnout dimensions
were represented as latent variables that were operationalized by five items (indicators) for exhaustion, four
items for cynicism, and six items for professional efficacy. We followed the same procedure but in a separate
analysis for the three engagement dimensions. These dimensions were also represented as latent variables and
were operationalized by five items for each of the dimensions: vigor, dedication and absorption. In the proposed
model, exhaustion is expected to correlate positively with cynicism and negatively with professional efficacy.
Cynicism is also expected to correlate negatively with professional efficacy. Regarding work engagement,
positive correlations between all three latent variables were expected.
To test Hypothesis 1, we conducted CFAs and we compared -for each national sample separately- the
proposed three-factor model of burnout (MBI-GS), where the underlying items loaded on the proposed factors,
with the following four alternative models: a) three alternative two-factor models (where, in each case, one of
the three factors consists of a combination of two burnout dimensions); and b) a one-factor model (where all
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
45
items are hypothesized to load on a single burnout latent factor). The same procedure was followed for testing
Hypothesis 2, where the proposed three-factor model (vigor, dedication, and absorption) of work engagement
(UWES) was compared with four alternative models for both national samples separately.
In addition, we conducted multigroup CFAs to test the measurement and structural invariance of each scale
across the two national samples (Vandenberg & Lance, 2000). Accordingly, we compared four nested models
for both scales: 1) to test for configural invariance (i.e. the same number of factors best represents the data for
both groups), we tested a multigroup model in which the only invariance constrain was that the exact same
parameters were tested for both national groups, while all these parameters were freely estimated (Model 1); 2)
to test for metric invariance (i.e. all participants, irrespective of their group membership, respond to the items of
the scale in the same way), we tested a model where factor loadings were also constrained to be equal across the
national groups (Model 2); 3) to test for scalar invariance (i.e. participants who have the same score on the latent
construct would obtain the same score on the observed variable irrespective of their group membership), we
imposed additional constraints to Model 2 by setting the item intercepts to be the same across groups as well
(Model 3); and 4) to test for structural invariance (i.e. all latent variables have the same scores and relationships
across groups), we tested a model (Model 4) where, next to equal factor loadings and item intercepts, all factor
variances and covariances were set to be equal across groups.
We used the following indices in order to assess model fit to the data: the chi-square (χ
2
) statistic and the
related degrees of freedom (df), the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). In general a
χ
2
/df less than 3.0 and GFI and CFI values above .90 indicate an acceptable fit (with >.95 being ideal; Brown,
2006). Further, RMSEA and SRMR values up to .08 indicate a reasonable fit, while values up to .05 indicate an
excellent fit to the data (Hu & Bentler, 1999). In order to compare nested models, we applied the chi-square
difference test, while in the multigroup analyses we used the CFI-difference (ΔCFI) index. Accordingly, a
decrease in the CFI value equal or higher than .01 indicates a significant decrement in model fit and lack of
invariance across groups (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002).
To test Hypothesis 3, we estimated latent mean differences. We chose “Dutch” to serve as the reference
group, and “Greek” to serve as the comparison group. We set the means of the latent factors to be fixed to zero
in the reference group, and to vary freely in the comparison group. Comparisons of latent means were based on
the critical ratio (CR) index (CR > ± 1.96 indicates statistical differences in means). A positive CR value
suggests that the comparison group has higher latent mean values than the reference group. It is important to
note that (full or partial) scalar invariance is a prerequisite first step in order to test for latent mean differences
(Vandenberg & Lance, 2000).
4 RESULTS
4.1 Descriptive Statistics
All scales of both instruments in both samples showed acceptable reliabilities (see Table 1). In the Dutch
sample, intercorrelations between the burnout dimensions and between the engagement dimensions were
significant and in the expected direction. Also, correlations between the burnout and engagement dimensions
were significant and in the expected direction. As for the Greek sample, correlations between the three
engagement dimensions were highly significant, but regarding the burnout dimensions, only exhaustion and
cynicism were correlated. Furthermore, the three engagement scales were moderately correlated with cynicism
and professional efficacy, but not with exhaustion. Likewise, the work engagement scales were stronger
interrelated than the burnout scales, in both national samples.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
46
Table 1: Means, Comparisons of Means, Standard Deviations, Correlations (for Greek Employees below
the Diagonal) and Internal Consistencies (Cronbach’s alphas) of the Burnout (EX, CY, PF) and
Engagement (VI, DE, AB) Scales in the Greek (N =206) and Dutch (N = 162) sample.
Greek
Dutch
MANOVA results
M
α
M
SD
α
F
EX
3.20
.88
2.30
1.36
.92
36.71***
CY
2.99
.84
1.90
1.24
.78
59.50***
PE
4.81
.85
4.22
0.89
.80
32.99***
VI
4.07
.85
3.86
1.08
.85
2.96
DE
3.84
.91
4.07
1.24
.92
2.62
AB
4.01
.86
3.69
1.06
.81
6.15*
Correlations
EX
CY
PE
VI
DE
AB
EX
.70**
-.37**
-.58**
-.58**
-.32**
CY
.50**
-.52**
-.63**
-.71**
-.51**
PE
.09
-.13
.65**
.70**
.53*
VI
-.00
-.18*
.59**
.81**
.80**
DE
.00
-.28**
.60**
.80**
.76**
AB
.03
-.14*
.55**
.87**
.76**
Note. EX = Exhaustion; CY = Cynicism; PE = Professional Efficacy; VI = Vigor; DE = Dedication, AB = Absorption ; * p
< .05 ; ** p < .01 ; *** p < .001.
4.2 Factor Structure and Invariance of Burnout across Greece and the Netherlands
Hypothesis 1 assumed the invariance of the three-factor structure of the MBI-GS (exhaustion, cynicism,
and professional efficacy) across Greece and the Netherlands. To test this hypothesis, we first conducted CFAs
for each national sample separately, and we compared the proposed three-factor model with four alternative
models, as described in the Method section. As shown on Table 2, the proposed three-factor model of burnout
had an acceptable fit to the data of both national samples. Also, chi-square
difference tests showed that the three-
factor structure
fitted better to the data of the Greek and Dutch samples than the alternative two and one-factor
models (see Table 2).
Table 2: Fit of Alternative Factor Models of the MBI-GS for the Greek (N = 206) and Dutch (N = 162)
Samples separately: Results of Confirmatory Factor Analyses.
Model
χ
2
df
χ
2
/ df
GFI
CFI
RMSEA
SRMR
Comparison
Δχ
2
df
Greece
I. 3-factor proposed
208.31
87
2.39
.88
.92
.08
.06
-
II. 2-factor (EX, CY+PE)
647.58
89
7.28
.63
.63
.18
.19
I - II
439.27*
2
III. 2-factor (CY, EX+PE)
659.78
89
7.41
.62
.62
.18
.19
I - III
451.47*
2
IV. 2-factor (PE, EX+CY)
448.47
89
5.04
.75
.76
.14
.11
I - IV
240.16*
2
V. 1- factor
892.96
90
9.92
.54
.46
.21
.20
I - V
684.65*
3
The Netherlands
I. 3-factor proposed
II. 2-factor (EX, CY+PE)
III. 2-factor (CY, EX+PE)
IV. 2-factor (PE, EX+CY)
V. 1- factor
147.61
248.33
355.08
236.96
410.28
87
89
89
89
90
1.70
2.79
3.99
2.66
4.56
.89
.79
.71
.82
.67
.95
.87
.79
.88
.75
.07
.11
.14
.10
.15
.06
.09
.13
.08
.13
-
I - II
I - III
I - IV
I - V
100.72*
207.47*
89.35*
262.67*
2
2
2
3
Note. df = degrees of freedom; GFI = goodness-of-fit Index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error
of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; * p < .001.
Next, we applied multigroup CFAs to test the invariance across the two national samples. Table 3 shows
that the model with no constraints (Model 1) fit well to the data, supporting configural invariance across the two
national groups. Next, the model where factor loadings were set to be equal across groups (Model 2) also fit
well to the data, while the ΔCFI test showed that the additional constraints that were imposed on this model did
not alter model fit. The next step was to test scalar invariance by setting the item intercepts to be equal across
groups (Model 3). Analyses did not support scalar invariance since the model in which item intercepts were also
set to be equal across groups did not fit well to the data and it caused a meaningful drop in fit (ΔCFI = .07).
Given that full scalar invariance was rejected, we tested whether partial invariance could be supported. In the
case of partial invariance, only a set of parameters is constrained to be invariant across groups, while the rest is
set to be estimated freely. Modifications indices showed that item 1 from the exhaustion scale, items 1, 2 and 5
from the cynicism scale, and item 6 from the professional efficacy scale lacked invariance. When the intercepts
of these items were freely estimated, partial invariance was still not supported, since Model 4 was found to be
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
47
different from Model 2 (ΔCFI = .03). Finally, structural invariance was also not supported (Model 5), since the
model where factor variances and covariances were set to me equal across groups did not fit well to the data. All
in all, these results partly support Hypothesis 1.
Table 3: Test of Invariance of the proposed Burnout (MBI-GS) and Work Engagement (UWES)
Structures across Greece and The Netherlands: Multigroup Confirmatory Factor Analysis (N = 368).
Model
χ
2
df
χ
2
/ df
CFI
RMSEA
SRMR
Comparison
ΔCFI
MBI-GS
1. No constraints
355.91
174
2.05
.93
.05
.06
-
2. Equal factor loadings
406.15
186
2.18
.92
.06
.07
1 - 2
.01
3. Equal Intercepts
604.34
201
3.00
.85
.07
.08
2 - 3
.07
4. Partial Scalar Invariance
487.65
196
2.49
.89
.06
.07
2 - 4
.03
5.Equal factor variances /
covariances
563.03
202
2.78
.87
.08
.15
4 - 5
.02
UWES
1. No constraints
553.29
174
3.18
.90
.08
.05
-
2. Equal factor loadings
567.35
186
3.05
.90
.08
.05
1 - 2
.00
3. Equal Intercepts
672.14
201
3.12
.88
.09
.05
2 - 3
.02
4. Partial Scalar Invariance
598.39
198
3.02
.90
.07
.05
2 - 4
.00
5. Equal factor variances /
covariances
626.70
204
3.07
.89
.08
.06
4 - 5
.01
Note. df = Degrees of Freedom; GFI = Goodness-of-Fit Index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square
Error of Approximation; SRMR = standardized root square residual.
4.3 Factor Structure and Invariance of Work Engagement in Greece and the Netherlands
To test Hypothesis 2, we followed the same procedure as for Hypothesis 1. Table 4 shows that the
proposed three-factor structure of work engagement fit reasonably well to the data of both samples. Note that
particularly the GFI and RMSEA are sensitive to sample size, and that the values for these indices should be
interpreted with some caution. Importantly, and in line with Hypothesis 2, chi-square
difference tests showed
that the proposed three-factor structure of work engagement fit significantly better to the data than all alternative
models tested in both samples (see Table 4).
Table 4: Fit of Alternative Factor Models of the UWES for the Greek (N = 206) and Dutch (N = 162)
Samples separately: Results of Confirmatory Factor Analyses.
Model
χ
2
df
χ
2
/ df
GFI
CFI
RMSEA
SRMR
Comparison
Δ χ
2
df
Greece
I. 3-factor proposed
298.79
87
3.43
.83
.91
.11
.05
-
-
-
II. 2-factor (VI, DE+AB)
313.07
89
3.52
.82
.90
.11
.05
I - II
14.28*
2
III. 2-factor (DE, VI+AB)
316.18
89
3.55
.83
.90
.11
.05
I - III
17.39*
2
IV. 2-factor (AB, VI+DE)
331.44
89
3.72
.80
.89
.12
.05
I - IV
32.65*
2
V. 1- factor
334.12
90
3.71
.80
.89
.12
.05
I - V
35.33*
3
The Netherlands
I. 3-factor proposed
254.49
87
2.93
.82
.90
.11
.06
-
-
-
II. 2-factor (VI, DE+AB)
278.12
89
3.12
.80
.89
.12
.07
I - II
23.63*
2
III. 2-factor (DE, VI+AB)
263.99
89
2.97
.81
.90
.11
.06
I - III
9.5*
2
IV. 2-factor (AB, VI+DE)
288.66
89
3.24
.80
.88
.12
.06
I - IV
34.17*
2
V. 1- factor
300.32
90
3.34
.79
.88
.12
.06
I - V
45.83*
3
Note. df = degrees of freedom; GFI = goodness-of-fit Index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error
of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; * p < .001.
Next, we estimated the invariance of the three-factor structure of the UWES by means of multigroup CFAs.
Table 3 shows that the model with no constraints (Model 1) fit very well to the data, suggesting that the
proposed three-factor structure of the UWES is invariant across countries. Metric invariance was also supported,
since setting the factor loadings to be equal across samples did not change the fit of the model (ΔCFI = .00;
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
48
Model 2). Full scalar invariance was not supported (Model 3), because adding constraints to the intercepts
across groups caused a significant decrease in the model fit (ΔCFI = .02). Examination of the modification
indices showed that item 4 of the vigor scale, item 3 of the dedication scale, and item 5 of the absorption scale
lacked invariance. Thus, we tested a partial invariance model (Model 4), where the intercepts of these items
were allowed to vary freely across groups. Results supported partial scalar invariance since the fit of the latter
model and the fit of the metric model did not vary significantly (ΔCFI = .00). Finally, results also supported
structural invariance (Model 5). Overall, results concerning the factor structure and the invariance of UWES
across Greece and the Netherlands fully support Hypothesis 2.
4.4 Differences in Burnout and Work Engagement between the Greek and Dutch samples
To test Hypothesis 3, we examined latent mean differences. Partial or full scalar invariance is a prerequisite
for testing differences between the means in the two national samples. Given that both full and partial scalar
invariance was not supported for the MBI-GS, latent mean differences could not be tested. As concerns the
UWES, for which partial scalar invariance was supported, analyses showed that the mean scores of Greek and
Dutch employees did not vary for any of the three dimensions of work engagement (for vigor: CR = 1.03, ns; for
dedication: CR = 1.15, ns; and for absorption: CR = .16, ns). These analyses reject Hypothesis 3. We further
tested potential differences between Greek and Dutch employee in the burnout and work engagement scores by
applying the less robust multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA). As shown in Table 1 and consistent with
Hypothesis 3, Greek employees scored significantly higher than the Dutch on the exhaustion [F (1, 368) =
36.71; p < .001] and cynicism [F (1, 368) = 59.50; p < .001] dimensions of burnout. However, unexpectedly,
Greek employees also scored significantly higher than Dutch employees on the professional efficacy dimension
of burnout [F (1, 368) = 32.98; p < .001], and the absorption dimension of engagement [F (1, 368) = 6.15; p <
.05). Thus, the MANOVA results partly support Hypothesis 3.
5 DISCUSSION
The present study examined the factor structure and invariance of the most frequently used instruments to
assess burnout (MBI-GS; Schaufeli et al., 1996) and work engagement (UWES; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010;
Schaufeli et al., 2002a; b) among heterogeneous samples of Greek and Dutch employees. In addition, we
compared employee levels of burnout and engagement in Greece and the Netherlands by estimating latent mean
differences. The innovative part of our study is that to our knowledge- this is the first attempt to investigate the
factor structure of the Greek version of the UWES. Furthermore, despite the fact that there is a significant
number of studies focusing on the psychometric properties of the MBS-GS and the UWES (for a review see,
Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010), we are not aware of any other study investigating full invariance (including scalar
invariance) across national or occupational groups.
5.1 Measuring Burnout
Hypothesis 1 stated that the three-factor structure of the MBI-GS (exhaustion, cynicism, and reduced
professional efficacy) would be invariant across Greece and the Netherlands. As expected and in line with
previous studies (e.g., Mäkikangas et al, 2011) the results of CFAs showed that the proposed three-factor model
of burnout fit well to the data in both national samples. Moreover, the three-factor model fit better to the data
than alternative two- and one-factor models, in which items of two or all three dimensions were collapsed.
Furthermore, the results of multigroup CFAs supported configural and metric invariance. Support for configural
invariance suggests that the three underlying factors (i.e. exhaustion, cynicism, and professional efficacy) best
represent the burnout concept both in Greece and in the Netherlands, while support for metric invariance reveals
that Greek and Dutch employees respond to the items of the MBI-GS in the same way. However, the results did
not support scalar invariance suggesting that the strength of the relationship between the constructs and the
specific scale items varies across the two groups. Also, failure to provide evidence for structural invariance
indicated that -to a certain degree- the strength of the relationship between the latent variables may be different
across groups (Tsaousis & Kazi, 2013).
Despite the fact that scalar and structural invariance were not supported, our results suggest that the MBI-
GS can be used in both Greece and The Netherlands, since responses to this burnout instrument do not seem to
be strongly influenced by specific characteristics of the employees working in these countries. The results show
that the phrasing of the items is adequate for cross-cultural comparisons, which is consistent with previous
findings in a Greek sample of employees (Demerouti et al., 2003), and findings among samples from Finland,
Norway, The Netherlands (Schutte et al., 2000), and Spain (Schaufeli et al., 2002a).
5.2 Measuring Work Engagement
Hypothesis 2 stated that the three-factor structure of the UWES (vigor, dedication and absorption) would
be invariant across Greece and The Netherlands. The results of CFAs confirmed that the proposed three-factor
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
49
structure of the UWES fitted reasonably well to the data of both national samples, and performed better than
alternative two- and one-factor models. However, it is interesting to note that although formal chi-square
difference tests revealed that the three-factor model was superior to alternative two-factor models, the fit indices
suggested that particularly the model collapsing the vigor and absorption items into one dimension fitted also
fairly well to the data. These findings are in line with the previous studies that have been conducted in The
Netherlands and Spain (Schaufeli et al., 2002a, 2002b), as well as the multinational study of Schaufeli et al.
(2006) who showed that the one-factor model of the UWES fitted reasonably well to the data just like the
proposed three-factor model. However, in the latter study the shorter, 9-item version of the UWES was used.
Nevertheless, Schaufeli et al. proposed that for practical purposes the total score of the UWES can be used as
the sole indicator of work engagement.
Importantly, the findings of the present study provided strong support for the invariance of the UWES
across Greece and the Netherlands since we found evidence for configural, metric, partial scalar, and structural
invariance. These findings suggest that the UWES is a very good instrument not only to measure engagement in
Greece and the Netherlands, but to also make comparisons across the two countries since Greek and Dutch
employees: a) recognize the same structure in engagement; b) respond to the items of the scale in a similar way;
3) show similar relationships between the latent constructs and the items; and 4) show similar relationship
among the latent variables. Another reason why these results are of particular importance is that none of the
previous studies on the factorial invariance of the UWES across nations or occupations has tested for and
supported scalar invariance (see Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010). Thus, this study adds to the literature by showing
for the first time that the UWES is an accurate tool if one is interested in comparing work engagement means
across groups.
5.3 Mean Differences
Hypothesis 3 stated that Greeks would be more burned out and less engaged than Dutch employees. To test
this hypothesis we examined latent mean differences for the UWES scale, since scalar invariance was supported
only for this scale (Vandenberg & Lance, 2000). The results of these analyses revealed that Greek and Dutch
employees did not differ significantly in their mean levels of vigor, dedication, and absorption. The less
elaborate results of the MANOVA suggested that Greek employees showed higher scores on exhaustion and
cynicism (the core dimensions of burnout), which is in line with previous findings (Demerouti et al., 2003).
However, contrary to expectations, Greeks also appeared to be more efficacious and more absorbed in their
work than Dutch. However, the findings of the MANOVAs should be dealt with caution since these were not
supported by the latent mean differences analyses (Tsaousis & Kazi, 2013).
5.4 Limitations of the Present Study and Suggestion for Future Research
The present study has certain limitations. First, although both samples are heterogeneous and cover a wide
range of different occupations, it is well conceivable that the Greek sample included slightly different
occupations than the Dutch. Future studies should try to recruit samples that are more comparable in terms of
occupations to enable more rigorous tests of the factor structure of burnout and engagement. Also, we have
restricted in comparing only two countries, while the study of at least three different national contexts could
allow more extensive cross-national comparisons. Another limitation of the study is its cross-sectional nature.
Thus, we cannot support structural invariance over time. Longitudinal studies are necessary to validate the
findings of the current study (for an example, see Mäkikangas et al., 2011). Finally, the present study restricted
itself to testing the factor structure of burnout and engagement, as well as comparing burnout and engagement
levels across Greece and The Netherlands, while it did not investigate similarities or differences as concerns the
psychological processes explaining employee well-being across countries.
5.5 Practical Implications
Burnout and engagement are considered to be significant indicators of employee well-being because they
relate to important outcomes for organizations (see Bakker et al., 2008; 2012; Halbesleben, 2010). Managers are
particularly interested in burnout because it relates to reduced employee performance (Bakker et al., 2008), as
well as in engagement that has been found to relate to financial profit (Xanthopoulou et al., 2009) and client
loyalty (Salanova et al., 2005). Therefore, practitioners are interested in reducing employee burnout and
increasing engagement. However, in order to do so, they first need to have valid instruments to measure these
states. The findings of this study contribute to organizational practice by showing that the MBI-GS (Schaufeli et
al., 1996) and the UWES (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010) are valid instruments that can provide robust estimations
of employee levels of burnout and work engagement, respectively. Importantly, the results of this study suggest
that particularly the UWES may provide not only accurate estimations of engagement but also accurate mean
comparisons across different sub-samples. Despite focusing only on two countries, the fact that we have
examined heterogeneous occupational samples allows a certain degree of generalizability since our findings
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
50
suggest that, irrespective of their occupation and their cultural background, employees perceive the scale items
in a similar way.
5.6 General Conclusion
The present study showed that the instruments that are broadly used to measure burnout (MBI-GS;
Schaufeli et al., 1996) and work engagement (UWES; Schaufeli & Bakker, 2010) are generally invariant across
Greece and the Netherlands, and thus comparisons between these two countries are meaningful, when these
instruments are used. Future cross-cultural comparative studies on the levels of burnout and engagement, with
carefully selected samples and longitudinal designs, will give us the opportunity to better explain positive and
negative aspects of employee well-being. For that, it would be important to test the causality of the differences
among countries by keeping the organizational context stable. Finally, an interesting project would be to test
differences among different countries at three levels of analyses: the cultural, the organizational, and the
individual level. Such a multilevel framework would allow a test of cross-level interactions and an investigation
of how these different levels determine individual employee burnout and engagement.
REFERENCES
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2002). Validation of the Maslach Burnout Inventory
General survey: An Internet study. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 15, 245-260.
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., Taris, T. W., Schaufeli, W. B., & Schreurs, P. J. G. (2003). A multigroup
analysis of the job demands-resources model in four home care organizations. International Journal of Stress
Management, 10, 16-38.
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Xanthopoulou, D. (2012). How do engaged employees stay engaged?
Cienca & Trabajo, 14, 15-21.
Bakker, A. B., Schaufeli, W.B., Leiter, M.P., & Taris, T.W. (2008). Work engagement: An emerging
concept in occupational health psychology. Work & Stress, 22, 187-200.
Bakker, A.B., Van Emmerik, H., & Van Riet, P. (2008). How job demands, resources, and burnout predict
objective performance: A constructive replication. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 21, 309-324.
Brown, T.A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cheung, G.W., & Rensvold, R.B. (2002). Evaluating goodness of fit indexes for testing measurement
invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9, 233-255.
Demerouti, E. Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2001). The Job Demands - Resources
model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 499-512.
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Vardakou, I., & Kantas, A. (2003).The convergent validity of two burnout
Instruments: A multitrait - multimethod analysis. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 18, 296-307.
Demerouti, E., Mostert, K., & Bakker, A.B. (2010). Burnout and work engagement: A thorough
investigation of the independency of both constructs. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 209-222.
Eurofound (2007). Fourth European working conditions survey. Publications Office of the European
Union, Luxembourg.
Eurofound (2012). Fifth European working conditions survey. Publications Office of the European Union,
Luxembourg.
González-Romá, V., Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B., & Lloret, S. (2006). Burnout and work engagement:
Independent factors or opposite poles? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 165-174.
Halbesleben, J.R.B. (2010). A meta-analysis of work engagement: Relationships with burnout, demands,
resources, and consequences. In A.B. Bakker & M.P. Leiter (Eds.), Work engagement: A handbook of essential
theory and research (pp. 102-117). New York: Psychology Press.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cut off criteria for the fit indexes in covariance structural analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Leiter, M. P., & Schaufeli, W.B. (1996).Consistency of the burnout construct across occupations. Anxiety,
Stress and Coping, 9, 229-243.
Luthans, F. (2002). Positive organizational behavior: Developing and maintaining psychological strengths.
Academy of Management Executive, 16, 57-72.
Mäkikangas, A., tinen, M., Kinnunen, U., & Pekkonen, M. (2011). Longitudinal factorial invariance of
the Maslach Burnout Inventory-General Survey among employees with job-related psychological health
problems. Stress & Health, 27, 347-352.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S.E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of Occupational
Behavior, 2, 99-113.
Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., & Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory manual (3rd ed.). Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (1997). The truth about burnout. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.
Despoina Xanthopoulou, Arnold Bakker, Aristotelis Kantas and Evangelia Demerouti
51
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (2008). Early predictors of job burnout and engagement. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 93, 498-512.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 397-
422.
Nerstad, C.G., Richardsen, A.M., & Martinussen, M. (2010). Factorial validity of the Utrecht Work
Engagement Scale (UWES) across occupational groups in Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51,
326-333.
Salanova, M., Agut, S., & Peiró, J.M. (2005). Linking organizational resources and work engagement to
employee performance and customer loyalty: The mediation of service climate. Journal of Applied Psychology,
90, 1217-1227.
Schaufeli, W.B., & Bakker, A.B. (2001). Werk en welbevinden: Naar een positieve benadering in de
Arbeids -en Gezondheidspsychologie [Work and well-being: Towards a positive Occupational Health
Psychology]. Gedrag & Organisatie, 14, 229-253.
Schaufeli, W.B., & Bakker, A.B. (2003). The Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES). Test manual.
Utrecht, The Netherlands: Department of Social & Organizational Psychology.
Schaufeli, W.B., & Bakker, A.B. (2004). Job demands, job resources and their relationship with burnout
and engagement: A multi - sample study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 1-23.
Schaufeli, W.B., & Bakker, A.B. (2010). Defining and measuring work engagement: Bringing clarity to the
concept. In A.B. Bakker & M.P. Leiter (Eds.), Work engagement: A handbook of essential theory and research
(pp. 10-24). New York: Psychology Press.
Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B., & Salanova, M. (2006).Work engagement with a short questionnaire: A
cross-national study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 701-716.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Greenglass, E. R. (2001). Introduction to special issue on burnout and health.
Psychology and Health, 16, 501-510.
Schaufeli, W. B., Leiter, M. P., Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory
General Survey. In Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., & Leiter, M. P. (Eds.), The Maslach Burnout Inventory-Test
manual (3rd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Schaufeli, W. B., Martinez., I. M., Marques-Pinto, A. M., Salanova, M., & Bakker, A. B. (2002a). Burnout
and engagement in university students. A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 464-
481.
Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B. (2002b). The measurement of
engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3,
71-92.
Schaufeli, W. B., & Van Dierendonck, D. (1993). The construct validity of two measures. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 14, 631-647.
Schutte, N., Toppinnen, S., Kalimo, R., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2000). The factorial validity of The Maslach
Burnout Inventory- General Survey across occupational groups and nations. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology, 73, 53-66.
Shirom, A. (2003). Job-related burnout: A review. In J. Campbell Quick, & L.E. Tetrick (Eds.), Handbook
of occupational health psychology (pp. 245-264). Washington, DC: Americal Psychological Association.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American
Psychologist, 55, 5-14.
Smith, P. B., Fischer, R., & Sale, N. (2001). Cross-Cultural Industrial Organisational Psychology. In C.L.,
Cooper, & I.T., Robertson (Eds), International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 16, 147-
193.
Taipale, S., Selander, K., Anttila, T., & Nätti, J. (2011). Work engagement in eight European countries:
The role of job demands, autonomy, and social support. The International Journal of Sociology and Social
Policy, 31, 486-504.
Taris, T. W., Schreurs, P. J. G., & Schaufeli, W. B. (1999). Construct validity of The Maslach Burnout
Inventory General Survey: A two - sample examination of its factor structure and correlates. Work and Stress,
13, 223-237.
Tsaousis, I., & Kazi, S. (2013). Factorial invariance and latent mean differences of scores on trait
emotional intelligence across gender and age. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 169-173.
Vandenberg, R.J., & Lance, C.E. (2000). A review and synthesis of measurement of the measurement
invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational
Research Methods, 3, 4-70.
Veenhoven, R. (2000). Freedom and happiness: A comparative study in forty-four nations in the early
1990’s. In Diener, E. & Suh, E.M. (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being. MIT Press.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
52
Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., & Schaufeli, W.B. (2009). Work engagement and
financial returns: A diary study on the role of job and personal resources. Journal of Occupational &
Organizational Psychology, 82, 183-200.