Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management, Volume 14, Issue 1, 2019
Functional meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance
in the workplace: The moderating role of basic psychological
needs satisfaction
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos
School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki
AUTH Campus, GR-54124
Tel: +30 2108811686
Email: papachristopouloskostas@gmail.com
Despoina Xanthopoulou
School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki
AUTH Campus, GR-54124
Tel: +30 2310997216
Email: dxanthopoulou@psy.auth.gr
Abstract
In this paper, we used self-determination theory to argue that the satisfaction of the basic psychological
needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness may act as a boundary condition that determines
when and how functional meaning of rewards (i.e., when individuals perceive work-related rewards as
informational or controlling) relates to interpersonal deviant behavior in the workplace. We
hypothesized that informational meaning of rewards will relate negatively and controlling meaning of
rewards will relate positively to interpersonal deviant behavior. Also, we expected that the former
relationship will be stronger when needs satisfaction is higher (vs. lower), and the latter relationship
will be weaker when needs satisfaction is higher (vs. lower). Hypotheses were tested by means of a
cross-sectional study with a heterogeneous sample of 265 Greek employees. Results of hierarchical
regression analyses showed that both controlling and informational meaning of rewards related
positively to deviant behavior. Also, relatedness need satisfaction moderated the relationships between
informational and controlling meaning of rewards with deviant behavior in a way that both facets of
rewards related positively to deviant behavior in conditions of lower relatedness need satisfaction,
while they were unrelated to deviant behavior in conditions of higher relatedness need satisfaction.
These results suggest that the role of the functional meaning of rewards for interpersonal deviance
depends on whether employees’ need of relatedness is satisfied or not in the workplace.
Keywords: functional meaning of rewards, interpersonal deviant behavior, psychological needs
satisfaction, self-determination theory
Acknowledgements: This research was supported by the act “Support of Postdoctoral Researchers” of
the European Program “Human Resources Development, Education and Lifelong Learning”, 2014
2020, which was implemented by the State Scholarships Foundation (IKY) and was co-funded by the
European Social Fund and the Hellenic State.
We thank Anais Thibault Landry for her useful comments during the preparation of this paper.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
2
1. INTRODUCTION
Pay is a core element of any human resources system and compensation constitutes one of the
most significant costs of an organization (Berber, Morley, Slavi, & Poór, 2017). Nevertheless,
empirical evidence on the role of compensation rewards for employee work-related behaviors has been
relatively scarce, and only during the past decade there have been several calls for more research on the
topic (Gagné & Forest, 2008; Gupta & Shaw, 2014; Thibault Landry, Gagné, Forest, Guerrero, Seguin
& Papachristopoulos, 2017a). This is mainly because the existing evidence on the effects of financial
incentives and rewards on employees is controversial (e.g., Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford, 2014). On the
one hand, studies have shown that financial rewards increase employees' efforts to contribute to
organizational goals (e.g., Condly, Clark, & Stolovitch, 2003). On the other hand, studies in laboratory
(e.g., Johnson, Dickson and Huitema, 2008) and organizational settings (e.g., Kuvaas, 2006)
highlighted the detrimental consequences that financial incentives may have for performance (Cerasoli
et al., 2014). Thus, there is still much debate around whether financial incentives are «good» or «bad»
for employee functioning at work.
In this paper, we follow the approach of Thibault Landry, Forest, Zigarmi, Houson, and Boucher
(2017b) on the functional meaning of financial rewards and propose that one way to understand the
inconsistent findings regarding the role of rewards is by recognizing that cash rewards can be perceived
differently by employees and determine their behavior, respectively. Hence, we account for the
distinction between informational (i.e., when rewards are perceived as supportive and encouraging of
individuals’ participation in their work) and controlling (i.e., when rewards are perceived as a mean to
control individuals' behavior; Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994) meaning of rewards in the context
of self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000), in order to explain how
rewards contribute to a pervasive workplace behavior, interpersonal deviance (Ferguson & Barry,
2011).
Also, we aim to understand under which conditions different meanings of rewards may be more or
less prominent for deviant behavior. To this end, we explore the moderating role of the satisfaction of
the basic psychological needs for autonomy (i.e., enhanced sense of volition), competence (i.e.,
increased sense of mastery), and relatedness (i.e., heightened sense of connection) at work (Ryan &
Deci, 2008) on the relationship between informational and controlling meaning of rewards and
interpersonal deviant behavior. Empirical evidence so far has mainly focused on whether rewards
satisfy or frustrate employees’ basic psychological needs (Del Vecchio & Wagner, 2011; Thibault et al,
2017a). Also, a recent study by Thibault Landry et al. (2017b) showed that basic psychological needs
mediate the relationship between functional meaning of rewards and organizational deviance. To shed
more light on the role that basic needs satisfaction plays in explaining the outcomes of functional
meaning of rewards, we argue that the degree to which basic needs are satisfied at work may determine
the strength of the relationship between functional meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviant
behavior. Figure 1 depicts the hypothesized model.
Figure 1. The hypothesized model
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
3
Our study contributes to the literature in the following ways. First, we contribute to the study of
cash rewards for interpersonal deviant behavior by recognizing that rewards are not received only as a
mere transaction. Rather, monetary rewards may have relational aspects that may play a role for
interpersonal behaviors such as interpersonal deviance. Focusing on the functional meaning of rewards
could help determine which perceptions of rewards are likely to increase or reduce the risk for such
behaviors. Second, by introducing the moderating role of psychological needs satisfaction on the
relationship between functional meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance we advance previous
studies on the direct impact of rewards on needs satisfaction. Our approach allows understanding
whether employees’ levels of needs satisfaction is a boundary condition that determines when
functional meaning of rewards are likely to promote deviant behavior. This has implications for theory
(since it unravels the conditions under which functional meaning of rewards may be more likely to
relate to deviant behaviors) and for practice (since satisfying employees’ basic needs organizations may
be proven able to mitigate the detrimental effects of rewards on employee behaviors).
2. LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1. Functional meaning of rewards and deviant work behavior: The SDT perspective
Workplace deviance refers to voluntary individual behaviors that violate organizational norms and
threaten the well-being of the organization and its employees (Bennett & Robinson, 2000). Workplace
deviance encompasses different organizational (e.g., working slowly, taking long breaks, internet
loafing) and interpersonal (e.g., harmful remarks, incivility) behaviors and it has been found to be
extremely costly for organizations worldwide (Ones, 2002). Interpersonal deviance -the focus of this
study- refers to deviant acts directed towards individuals within the organization, such as managers,
coworkers, and subordinates (Bennett & Robinson, 2000). Interpersonal deviance has detrimental
consequences for both target individuals and organizations since it is associated with lower
psychological well-being and increased stress (e.g., Cortina, Magley, Williams, & Langhout, 2001), as
well as reduced affective commitment, and heightened turnover intentions (Hershcovis & Barling,
2010). Research has also shown that negative interpersonal behaviors and conflicts associate positively
with reduced information-sharing and task performance (Kammeyer-Mueller, Simon & Rich, 2012;
Porath & Pearson, 2010).
While previous research explored the relation between cash rewards and performance (Cerasoli et
al., 2014), the relationship between financial rewards and interpersonal deviance is relatively under-
studied. Nevertheless, there are indications that, when organizations focus on cash rewards, employees
are prompted to adopt an end-justifies-the-means” mentality (Thibault Landry et al., 2017b), and may
manifest competitive behaviors or aggressive actions towards others (Garcia, Tor, & Schiff,
2013).Thus, it is important to understand whether and when the impact of financial incentives is
relevant for interpersonal deviant behaviors.
Research evidence suggests that offering money does not always constitute the best way to
enhance optimal work behaviors (Deci, 1972; Gagné & Forest, 2008; Ryan & Deci, 2011;
Vansteenkiste, Neyrinck, Niemiec, Soenens, Witte, & Broeck, 2007). This is because the boosting
effect of financial rewards seems to affect mainly the quantity and not always the quality of employees'
behavior, while it seems to be merely temporary (e.g., Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999a,1999b). It has
been argued that the role of financial rewards for work behaviors stems from a motivational shift,
whereby individuals may engage in an activity either for the external, financial gain, or for its own sake
and enjoyment (Frey & Jegen, 2001; Krug & Braver, 2014).Thus, it is of relevance to use motivational
theories to understand the meaning employees give to their cash rewards and how this determines their
behavior. In this context, SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2008) posits that financial incentives
take on an informational meaning when they are perceived as supportive and encouraging of
individuals’ participation in their work, leading to autonomous motivation and optimal functioning at
work (Moller & Deci, 2014). In contrast, rewards take on a controlling meaning when they are
presented as oppressive and aiming to control individuals' behavior (Deci, Connell, & Ryan 1989, Deci
et al., 1994).
Research in a variety of contexts (e.g., sales, sports, education) has shown that stressing financial
incentives as a mean to achieve a goal can increase problematic work behaviors such as dishonesty,
manipulative sales, cheating in order to get the reward offered (Aguinis, Joo, & Gottredson, 2013;
Kouchaki, Smith-Crowe, Brie, & Sousa, 2013; Madhani, 2014; Thibault Landry et al., 2017b). When
employees are offered rewards in a pure transactional way, they may be more prone to interpersonal
deviant behavior. As Festinger (1954) suggested, people in a unidirectional push upwards (i.e., a
controlling condition), are likely to react with competitive behavior to protect their threatened
superiority or reduce others’ success. Thus, a context of controlling rewards that promotes competition
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
4
and transactional culture may motivate interpersonal deviant behavior (Gläser & Van Quaquebeke,
2017). In contrast, based on social exchange theory (Blau, 1964), we argue that when employees
perceive their rewards from the organization as informational (i.e., supportive and encouraging of their
participation and effort), they are more likely to reciprocate for this recognition by avoiding behaviors
that may harm the organization. Based on the above, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 1: Informational meaning of rewards will relate negatively with interpersonal deviant
behavior.
Hypothesis 2: Controlling meaning of rewards will relate positively with interpersonal deviant
behavior.
2.2. The moderating role of basic psychological needs satisfaction
A central assumption of SDT (Deci & Ryan, 2000) is that employee attitudes and behaviors are
dependent on whether the activities they engage in at work contribute to the satisfaction of the three
basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness. These basic psychological needs
can be more or less fulfilled depending on the work contexts, and greater satisfaction of these needs
leads to better outcomes (Ryan & Deci, 2008), including greater task performance, enjoyment, and
vigor, and less organizational deviance (e.g., De Cooman, Stynen, Van den Broeck, Sels, & De Witte,
2013; Olafsen, et al., 2015; Vansteenkiste et al., 2007; Van de Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Witte,
Soenens, & Lens, 2010). To satisfy the need for autonomy, individuals must feel that the activity they
are pursuing is congruent with their personal values and have a sense of volition when exercising it
(Ryan & Deci, 2008). To satisfy the need for competence, individuals should feel that they have all
skills required so as to influence their environment and achieve their goals (Deci & Ryan, 2000). To
satisfy the need for relatedness, individuals must feel that they are and can be emotionally connected to
other people in their (work) environment (Baumeister & Leary, 1995).
Two recent studies (Thibault Landry et al.,2017a; 2017b) have shown that rewards, when used in a
way that acknowledge employees' skills and evoke appreciation (i.e., gain informational meaning),
relate to a higher satisfaction of employees’ psychological needs, that in turn, associates positively to
motivation and commitment and negatively to turnover intentions. Despite these findings suggesting
that basic need satisfaction mediates the relationship between functional meaning of rewards and
employee behaviors, in the present study, we argue that the satisfaction of basic needs may also
moderate this relationship.
In line with social exchange theory (Blau, 1964), when employees perceive their rewards as
informational and at the same time feel that their needs for autonomy, relatedness and competence are
satisfied at work, they are more likely to feel privileged and favored by the organization thus,
reciprocating by avoiding interpersonal deviant behaviors. Thus, the satisfaction of the three basic
needs will boost the negative relationship between informational meaning of rewards and interpersonal
deviant behavior. Also, we expect that the satisfaction of the three basic needs will buffer the positive
relationship between controlling meaning of rewards and deviant behavior. When employees perceive
their rewards as controlling but their basic needs are satisfied at work, they will be less likely to exhibit
deviant behaviors because their sense of superiority may not be highly threatened in this condition
(Festinger, 1954). This is because employees who feel competent to achieve their desired goals and feel
that they owe a sense of volition and a sense of belonging will not be substantially influenced by the
coercive meaning controlling rewards elicit, which forces them to endorse behaviors (e.g., competing)
that are purely accessory to getting the reward. In line with this theorizing and empirical evidence, we
hypothesize:
Hypothesis 3a: Need satisfaction will moderate the negative relationship between informational
meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance in the workplace in a way that the negative
relationship will be stronger when needs satisfaction is higher (vs. lower; see Figure 2).
Hypothesis 3b: Need satisfaction will moderate the positive relationship between controlling
meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance in the workplace, in a way that the positive
relationship will be weaker when needs satisfaction is higher (vs. lower; see Figure 3).
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
5
Figure 2. The hypothesized two-way interaction effect between informational meaning of rewards
and basic needs satisfaction in explaining interpersonal deviant behaviour
Figure 3. The hypothesized two-way interaction effect between controlling meaning of rewards
and basic needs satisfaction in explaining interpersonal deviant behaviour
3. METHOD
3.1. Participants and procedure
This study was conducted in Greece from January to March 2018. Employees working in private
and public organizations were approached with the snowball method. They were informed about the
research aims and the study procedure and they were reassured that participation was voluntary and
that the data they will provide with their answers will be kept confidential. Interested employees could
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
6
either complete an online, electronic version or a paper-and-pencil version of the questionnaire. In the
case of the online data collection, employees who agreed to participate, received the link to the
electronic questionnaire via email. Paper-and-pencil questionnaires were provided to employees by
research assistants and were completed right after participants were informed about the study.
One-hundred and seventy questionnaires were completed online, and 95 questionnaires were
completed via paper and pencil. The final sample consisted of 265 Greek employees, 49% of whom
were working in the public sector and 43% were employed in the private sector. The sample consisted
of 168 women and 97 men with a mean age of 37.20 (SD = 9.60) years. Most participants (83%)
worked full-time. Participants’ average job tenure was 10.08 (SD = 8.87) years. Participants’ average
individual annual income was 14.240,00 (SD = 8.790) euros.
3.2. Measures
All scales were administered in the Greek language. Original scales were translated from English
to Greek using the method of back translation.
Functional meaning of rewards. Informational meaning of rewards was assessed using four items
that were adapted by Thibault Landry and colleagues (2017b) from the Perceived Autonomy Support
Scale for Exercise Settings (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2007). An example item is: “My boss displays
confidence in my ability to work, when he gives me cash rewards.” The scale was reliable (Cronbach’s
alpha = .94). Controlling meaning of rewards was assessed with three items from the Controlling
Coach Behavior Scale (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, Bosch, & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2011). In the
sports setting, this subscale is used to measure the extent to which coaches employ external rewards to
motivate their athletes. The three items were adapted to the work setting (e.g., “My boss only uses cash
rewards so that I stay focused on tasks during work”) by Thibault Landry and colleagues (2017b). The
scale was reliable with Cronbach’s alpha = .89. All items of both subscales were evaluated on a 7-point
scale ranging from (1) = strongly disagree to (7) = strongly agree.
Psychological needs satisfaction was measured with the Work-Related Basic Need Satisfaction
Scale developed by Van den Broeck et al. (2010). This scale includes 16 items that measure all three
facets of psychological need satisfaction. Namely, autonomy need satisfaction was measured with six
items (e.g., “I feel free to do my job the way I think it could best be done”; Cronbach’s alpha = .81).
Competence need satisfaction was assessed with four items (e.g., “I feel competent at my job”;
Cronbach’s alpha = .84). Finally, relatedness need satisfaction was assessed with six items such as: “I
feel part of a group at work” (Cronbach’s alpha =.83). Participants rated the scale items using a 5-point
scale ranging from (1) = totally disagree to (5) = totally agree. Three items of the autonomy need
satisfaction sub-scale and three items of the relatedness need satisfaction sub-scale that were negatively
framed were reversed-coded so that high scores indicated higher need satisfaction.
Interpersonal deviance was assessed with the 7-item scale developed by Bennett and Robinson
(2000). An example item is 'During the last 3 months how often did you act rudely toward someone at
work?’ Items were answered on a scale ranging from (1) = never to (7) = every day. The scale was
reliable (Cronbach’s alpha = .81).
3.3. Strategy of analysis
To test for the proposed main and moderating effects we performed hierarchical regression
analyses. Main and interaction effects were tested simultaneously in separate analyses for each
meaning of reward and each type of need satisfaction. Thus, hypothesized effects were tested in six sets
of analyses. Predictor and moderating variables were standardized prior to calculating the cross-product
interaction terms (Aiken & West, 1991). In the first step of the regression, we controlled for age,
gender, and sector. In the second step, the predictor (each of the two functional meaning of rewards)
and moderator (satisfaction of each of the three needs) variables were entered to the regression
equation, followed by the interaction of the two in the third step. The incremental variance accounted
for by the interaction term represents the effect size of the interaction. Significant interaction effects
were probed with the simple effects approach and were plotted by using +/- 1 SD of the moderating
variables (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006).
4. RESULTS
4.1. Descriptive results
Table 1 presents means, standard deviations and correlations between the study variables. In
contrast to expectations, both informational meaning (r = .20, p < .01) and controlling meaning (r = .36,
p < .01) of rewards were positively associated with interpersonal deviance. As concerns the satisfaction
of basic needs, only relatedness need satisfaction correlated negatively and significantly with
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
7
interpersonal deviance in the workplace (r = -.14, p < .05). Age correlated negatively with
informational meaning of rewards (r = -.12, p < .05), and with autonomy (r = .17, p < .01) and
competence need satisfaction (r = .19, p < .01). Gender correlated negatively with interpersonal
deviance (r = -.29, p < .01), indicating that women reported lower interpersonal deviance. Also, women
reported lower levels of controlling meaning of rewards (r = -.16, p < .01). Job sector (public vs.
private) correlated positively and significantly with both aspects of the functional meaning of rewards.
Participants’ annual income did not correlate significantly with any of the study variables. Therefore,
we controlled for age, gender, and sector in all subsequent analyses. Although job tenure correlated
significantly with autonomy and relatedness need satisfaction, it also correlated highly with age (r =
.78, p < .01). Thus, to avoid multicollinearity issues, we controlled only for age in our analyses, and not
for job tenure.
Table 1 shows that informational and controlling meaning of rewards correlated highly with each
other (r = .74, p < .01). To empirically support that these two factors are related but distinct, we
performed Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFAs) and we compared a two-factor model (where each
meaning of rewards was represented as a latent factor with the respective items as indicators and where
the two latent factors were allowed to covary), to a one-factor model (where all items of both scales
loaded on one latent factor). Results provided support for the empirical distinctiveness of the two
constructs since the two-factor model fit significantly better to the data than the one-factor model [Δχ2
(1) = 184.29, p < .001].
4.2. Main analyses
According to Hypothesis 1 informational meaning of rewards was expected to relate negatively
with interpersonal deviant behavior. As shown in Models 2 and 3 of Table 2, Hypothesis 1 was rejected
since in all three tests, informational meaning of rewards related positively and significantly with
deviant behavior = .20, p < .01). According to Hypothesis 2, controlling meaning of rewards was
expected to relate positively with deviant behavior. Results provided support for Hypothesis 2, since in
all three tests of this relationship, controlling meaning of rewards related positively and significantly
with deviant behavior (.29 < β < .34, p < .01; see Models 2 and 3 of Table 2).
According to Hypothesis 3a, the satisfaction of the psychological needs for autonomy, relatedness
and competence was expected to moderate the negative relationship between informational meaning of
rewards and interpersonal deviance in the workplace in a way that the negative relationship would be
stronger when needs satisfaction was higher (vs. lower). As shown in Table 2 (Model 3), only the
interaction effect of informational meaning of rewards with relatedness need satisfaction was
significant (β = -.13, p < .05) explaining 2% of additional variance in deviant behavior. This significant
interaction effect is depicted on Figure 4. The simple slopes test showed that the relationship between
informational meaning of rewards and deviant behavior was positive and significant only when
relatedness need satisfaction was lower (-1SD: estimate = .22, t = 3.69, p < .01), while the relationship
was not significant when relatedness need satisfaction was higher (+1SD: estimate = -.01, t = -.24, p =
.82). These results are not in line with Hypothesis 3a, since they suggest that higher relatedness need
satisfaction buffers the positive relationship between informational meaning of rewards and deviant
behavior.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
8
Figure 4. The two-way interaction effect between informational meaning of rewards and
relatedness need satisfaction in explaining interpersonal deviant behaviour
Figure 5. The two-way interaction effect between controlling meaning of rewards and relatedness
need satisfaction in explaining interpersonal deviant behaviour
According to Hypothesis 3b, the satisfaction of the basic psychological needs was expected to
moderate the positive relationship between controlling meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance
in the workplace, in a way that this positive relationship would be weaker when needs satisfaction was
higher (vs. lower). As shown in Table 2 (Model 3), again, only the interaction effect of controlling
meaning of rewards with relatedness need satisfaction was significant = -.22, p < .001) explaining
4% of additional variance in deviant behavior. This significant interaction effect is depicted on Figure
5. The simple slopes test showed that the relationship between controlling meaning of rewards and
deviant behavior was positive and significant only when relatedness need satisfaction was lower (-1SD:
estimate = .39, t = 6.45, p < .001), while the relationship was not significant when relatedness need
satisfaction was higher (+1SD: estimate = -.03, t = -.48, p = .65). These results provide support for
Hypothesis 3b since they suggest that high relatedness need satisfaction mitigates the positive
relationship between controlling meaning of rewards and deviant behavior. It is important to note that
results were similar, even when control variables were excluded from the analyses.
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
9
Table 1: Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations between the study variables (N = 265)
Variables
M
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
1. Controlling meaning of rewards
2.21
-
2. Informational meaning of rewards
2.68
.74
**
-
3. Relatedness need satisfaction
3.63
-.06
-.04
-
4. Autonomy need satisfaction
3.08
.02
.03
.35
**
-
5. Competence need satisfaction
4.10
-.09
.01
.18
**
.23
**
-
6. Interpersonal deviance
1.87
.36
**
.20
**
-.14
*
-.01
-.04
-
7. Age
37.90
-.03
-.12
*
.11
.17
**
.19
**
-.00
-
8. Tenure
10.08
.01
-.06
.04
.14
*
.22
**
.12
.78
**
-
9. Gender (1 = Male; 2 = Female)
-
-.16
**
-.07
.05
.03
-.19
-.29
**
-.05
-.09
-
10. Sector (1 = Public; 2 = Private)
-
.15
*
.17
**
-.03
-.00
.00
.01
-.29
**
-.27
**
-.06
-
11. Annual Individual Income (Euro)
14240,00
.03
.00
.05
.12
.06
.01
.32
**
.19
**
-.12
-.17
**
Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
10
Table 2: Main and Interaction Effects Explaining Interpersonal Deviance (N = 265)
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Variable
B
SE
β
B
SE
β
B
SE
β
Gender
.58
.17
-.30**
-.56
.11
-.29**
-.56
.11
-.29**
Age
-.00
.00
-.03
-.00
-.00
-.01
.00
.00
-.00
Sector
-.03
-.09
-.02
-.07
.09
-.05
-.06
.09
-.04
Autonomy need satisf.
.00
.07
.00
.01
.07
.00
Inform. Meaning of Rewards
.10
.07
.20**
.10
.03
.20**
Inform. Meaning x Autonomy
.08
.05
.08
R
2
F change
.09
8.26**
.13
5.69**
.14
1.96
Gender
-.56
.11
-.29**
-.55
.17
-.29**
Age
-.00
-.00
-.00
-.00
.00
.00
Sector
-.06
.09
-.05
-.06
.09
-.05
Inform. Meaning of Rewards
.10
.03
.20**
.10
.03
.20**
Competence need satisf.
-.06
.09
-.04
-.07
.09
-.04
Inform. Meaning x Competence
-.01
.05
-.02
R
2
F change
.13
5.95**
.13
.078
Gender
-.55
.11
-.28**
-.51
.11
-.26**
Age
.00
.00
.00
-.00
.00
-.01
Sector
-.06
.08
-.05
-.07
.08
-.05
Inform. Meaning of Rewards
.10
.03
.20**
.10
.03
.20**
Relatedness need satisf.
-.13
.07
-.12
-.13
.06
-.11
Inform. Meaning x Relatedness
-.11
.05
-.13*
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
11
R
2
F change
.14
7.77 **
.16
5.11*
Gender
-.47
.11
-.25**
-.48
.11
-.25**
Age
-.00
.00
-.03
-.00
.00
-.03
Sector
-.10
.08
-.07
-.10
.08
-.07
Control. Meaning of Rewards
.21
.04
.33**
.21
.04
.34**
Autonomy need satisf.
.00
.07
.00
.01
.07
.00
Control. Meaning x Autonomy
.08
.05
.09
R
2
F change
.19
16.53***
.20
2.36
Gender
-.47
.11
-.24**
-.47
.11
-.25**
Age
-.00
.00
-.02
-.00
.00
-.03
Sector
-.09
.08
-.07
-.97
.08
-.07
Control. Meaning of Rewards
.20
.04
.33**
.20
.03
.33**
Competence need satisf.
-.00
.08
-.00
-.00
.08
-.00
Control. Meaning x Competence
.01
.05
.01
R
2
F change
.20
16.52***
.20
2.36
Gender
-.46
.11
-.24***
-.41
.10
-.21***
Age
.00
.00
-.02
-.00
.00
-.04
Sector
-.09
.08
-.07
-.10
.08
-.07
Control. Meaning
.20
.04
.33***
.18
.03
.29***
Relatedness need satisf.
-.15
.06
-.14*
Control.Meaning x Relatedness
-.21
.06
-.22***
R
2
F change
.21
18.38***
.25
14.76***
Note. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
12
5. DISCUSSION
The main aim of this study was to investigate how the functional meaning (i.e., informational or
controlling) of rewards relates to interpersonal deviant behavior in the workplace, and whether the
satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness moderate these
relationships. Based on the main assumptions of SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000), we
hypothesized that informational meaning of rewards will relate negatively, while controlling meaning
of rewards will relate positively to interpersonal deviant behavior. Also, based on social exchange
theory (Blau, 1964), we expected that the satisfaction of the basic psychological needs will moderate
these relationships. As expected, results showed that controlling meaning of rewards related positively
with interpersonal deviance at work. However, contrary to expectations, informational meaning of
rewards was also found to associate positively with interpersonal deviance. Furthermore, relatedness
(but not autonomy and competence) need satisfaction buffered the positive links between both
informational and controlling meaning of rewards with interpersonal deviance in the workplace. In
what follows, we discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings.
The findings of this study complement previous empirical evidence on the importance of the
functional meaning of rewards for understanding work-related behaviors (e.g., Deci et al, 1999, Deci,
Olafsen, & Ryan, 2017, Ryan & Deci, 2017, Thibault Landry et al., 2017b). As recent empirical studies
have shown (e.g., Kushlev, Dunn, & Lucas, 2015, Thibault Landry et al., 2016), money is more likely
to be a symbol that takes on different functional meanings that result in differential behaviors at work.
For instance, Thibault Landry and colleagues (2017b) showed that only controlling (but not
informational) meaning of rewards related positively with organizational deviant behavior. However,
and against our expectations, we found that both informational and controlling meaning of rewards to
associate positively with interpersonal deviance.
These unexpected findings may be attributed to the context, where the study took place -that is
Greece in the aftermath of the global financial crisis. Greece has been amongst the countries that were
hit harder by the economic recession. According to Eurofound (2019), wages have been cut down in
both the public and the private sectors, and Greece has been the only EU country where a 22% nominal
reduction of the minimum wage (32% for young people under 25) was imposed in 2012. Furthermore,
youth unemployment was 43.6% in 2017, while the EU average for this group was 16.8%. In such an
insecure environment, it may be quite ambiguous for employees to distinguish between rewards as a
signal of appreciation and rewards as a signal of instrumentality and coercion, while any perceived sign
of reward may evoke competitive tendencies that can lead to interpersonal and organizational deviance
(Garcia et al., 2013). Also, in contexts where bonuses and pay-for-performance schemes are not
widespread (such as in Greece), any reference to additional pay may induce social comparison
processes and counterproductive interpersonal behavior. The fact that our findings are different to those
of Thibault Landry and colleagues (2017b), who conducted their study in the Canadian context that was
affected less by the global financial crisis, implies that future studies should investigate whether the
financial environment moderates the relationship between functional meaning of rewards and
interpersonal deviance in the workplace.
Next to the main effects of functional meaning of rewards, the results of this study suggest that the
way personal and professional relationships are structured and perceived in the workplace moderate the
relationship between functional meaning of rewards and deviant behavior. In this deed, it seems that
basic need satisfaction, and particularly relatedness need satisfaction, apart from being an explanatory
mechanism between rewards and work-related outcomes (Olafsen et al., 2015), may also function as a
boundary condition that mitigates the positive relationship between controlling and informational
meaning of rewards and interpersonal deviance. These findings can be explained by SDT (Ferris,
Brown, & Heller, 2009; Deci & Ryan, 2000), which suggests that when basic needs are satisfied,
individuals have more emotional and cognitive resources to self-regulate their behavior in the
workplace. Also, when employees fulfil their natural tendency to seek for coherent and meaningful
relationships (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), their need for social comparison might not be of high
importance to them when presented with rewards, preventing interpersonal deviance (Garcia et al.,
2013).
Our findings contribute to a more concrete understanding of the role of functional meaning of
rewards for workplace behaviors in the context of SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2017) in at least two ways. First,
our results extend previous findings that provided support for the mediating role of autonomy and
competence need satisfaction in the relationship between functional meaning of rewards and
organizational deviance (Thibault Landry et al., 2017b), by highlighting that the satisfaction of the
basic psychological needs in the workplace and particularly the satisfaction of the need of relatedness
may also buffer the detrimental impact of functional meaning of rewards on interpersonal deviance. In
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
13
this way, our findings suggest that basic need satisfaction is not only the underlying psychological
mechanism that explains the link between functional meaning of rewards and employee behaviors but
may also be the boundary condition that determines when the outcomes of functional meaning of
rewards may be particularly detrimental. Second, taking into consideration that previous studies
focused solely on the role of competence and autonomy need satisfaction in rewards-related research
(e.g., Bureau et al., 2018, Gerhart & Fang, 2015; Moller & Deci, 2014; Thibault Landry et al., 2017b),
our study highlights the important role of relatedness need satisfaction. It may be the case that
autonomy and competence need satisfaction may better explain why rewards lead to individual
outcomes such as task performance, since they are directly targeted by current compensation practices
(Del Vecchio & Wagner, 2011; Houlfort, Koestner, Joussement, & Lekes, 2002; Thibault et al, 2017b).
Nevertheless, when it comes to work outcomes related to employees’ relationships), relatedness need
satisfaction may play a more crucial role.
5.1. Practical implications
Our research has several managerial implications in a period, where collaboration and creativity
are significant prerequisites for organizational growth (George, 2007), and interpersonal deviance may
severely undermine these work characteristics. In contrast to expectations and previous findings
(Thibault Landry et al., 2017b), our findings suggest that both informational and controlling meaning
of rewards may elicit deviant behaviors in employees but only when employees feel that their need to
relate with others at work is not satisfied. This finding has important implications for practice because
it suggests that it is not how employees perceive their rewards per se that determines their behavior.
Rather, the work environment and whether it satisfies their basic needs is the boundary condition that
determines whether and in which way functional meaning of rewards form employee behavior. Based
on our findings, organizations should make sure to promote work environments that facilitate the
satisfaction of the basic psychological needs and particularly, the need for relatedness, since the
satisfaction of this need buffers the positive relationship of functional meaning of rewards on employee
deviant behaviors. To this end, specific examples of workplace interventions have been developed in
the context of SDT that have been proven effective in promoting basic need satisfaction in the
workplace (e.g. Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2004, Hardré & Reeves, 2009; Williams et al., 2014
5.2. Limitations and future research
There are several limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the study results. First,
the study was cross-sectional, and thus one should be wary of making any causal inferences. Future
studies that employ longitudinal designs may investigate whether the relationship between functional
meaning of rewards and deviant behavior is long-lasting, and whether relatedness need satisfaction may
also mitigate this relationship over the course of time. Moreover, to overcome the inherent
shortcomings of self-reporting, it would be important to conduct research that uses other-rating of
employee behaviors. Moreover, since rewards have effects at both the individual- and the team-level
(e.g., through social comparison), future research may want to employ multi-level approaches to shed
light on the connection between competitive climate, team cohesiveness and individual perceptions of
rewards. Future research should also take into account personal characteristics such as extrinsic or
intrinsic values (Kasser, 2016), financial contingency of self-worth (Park, Ward, & Naragon-Gainey,
2017) or trait competitiveness (Gläser & Van Quaquebeke, 2017) that may associate with the way
employees perceive and make projections related to the rewards presented to them.
6. CONCLUSION
Based on the main assumptions of SDT about the functional meaning of rewards (Ryan & Deci,
2000), in this study we showed that both informational and controlling meaning of rewards relate
positively to deviant behavior but only when the need for relatedness is not satisfied in the work
environment. In contrast, the positive relationship between functional meaning of rewards and deviant
behavior was non-existent when employees’ need of relatedness is satisfied. These results extend
theorizing on the role of rewards in the context of SDT by putting forward the moderating role of basic
needs satisfaction on the rewards-behaviors relationship, and by unraveling the boundary conditions
under which functional meaning of rewards may be more likely to relate to deviant behaviors. Our
findings imply that organizations should focus on how to support a climate of relatedness among
employees since this may mitigate the detrimental effects of rewards on employee behaviors.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
14
REFERENCES
Aguinis, H., Joo, H., & Gottredson, R. K. (2013). What monetary rewards can and cannot do: How
to show employees the money. Business Horizons, 56, 241-249.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury
Park: Sage.
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need satisfaction: A motivational basis of
performance and well-being in two work settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34,
2045-2068.
Balkins, D. B., Roussel, P., & Werner, S. (2015). Performance contingent pay and autonomy:
Implications for facilitating extra-role creativity. Human Resource Management Review, 25, 384-
395.
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., Bosch, J. A., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011).
Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: The role of interpersonal control and
psychological need thwarting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1459-1473.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a
fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2000). Development of a measure of workplace deviance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 85, 349360.
Berber, N., Morley, J.M., Slavi, A., & Poór, J. (2017). Management compensation systems in Central
and Eastern Europe: a comparative analysis. The International Journal of Human Resource
Management, 28, 1661-1689.
Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Bureau, J., Mageau, G., Alexandre M., Gagné, M., Forest, J., Papachristopoulos, K., Lucas, A.,
Thibault Landry, A., & Parenteau, C. (2018). Promoting autonomy to reduce employee deviance:
The mediating role of identified motivation. International Journal of Business and Management,
13, 61-71.
Cerasoli, C. P., Nicklin, J. M., & Ford, M. T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives
jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 980-1008.
Condly, S. J., Clark, R. E., & Stolovitch, H. D. (2003). The effects of incentives on workplace
performance: A meta-analytic review of research studies 1. Performance Improvement Quarterly,
16, 46-63.
Cortina, L.M., Magley, V.J., Williams, J.H., & Langhout, R.D. (2001). Incivility in the workplace:
Incidence and impact. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 64-80.
De Cooman, R., Stynen, D., Van den Broeck, A., Sels, L., & De Witte, H. (2013). How job
characteristics relate to need satisfaction and autonomous motivation: Implications for work effort.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 1342-1352.
Deci, E. L. (1972). The effects of contingent and non-contingent rewards and controls on intrinsic
motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 217-229.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New
York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The" what" and" why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-
determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268
Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-determination in a work organization. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 74, 580-590.
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-
determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119-142.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999a). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining
the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627-668.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999b). The undermining effect is a reality after all
Extrinsic rewards, task interest, and self-determination: Reply to Eisenberger, Pierce, and
Cameron (1999) and Lepper, Henderlong, and Gingras (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 692-
700.
Konstantinos Papachristopoulos and Despoina Xanthopoulou
15
Deci, E. L., Olafsen, A. H., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). Self-determination theory in work organizations:
The state of a science. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational
Behavior, 4, 226-242.
DelVecchio, S., & Wagner, J. (2011). Motivation and monetary incentives: A closer look. Journal of
Management and Marketing Research, 7, 1-13.
Eurofound (2019). Living and working in Europe 20152018. Publications Office of the European
Union, Luxembourg.
Ferguson, M., & Barry, B. (2011). I know what you did: The effects of interpersonal deviance on
bystanders. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16, 80-94.
Ferris, D. L., Brown, D. J., & Heller, D. (2009). Organizational supports and workplace deviance: The
mediating role of organization-based self-esteem. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 108, 279286.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.
Frey, B. S., & Jegen, R. (2001). Motivational interactions: Effects on behaviour. Annales d’Économie
et de Statistique, 63, 131-153.
Gagné, M., & Forest, J. (2008). The study of compensation systems through the lens of self-
determination theory: Reconciling 35 years of debate. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie
Canadienne, 49, 225-232.
Garcia, S. M., Tor, A., & Schiff, T. M. (2013). The psychology of competition: A social comparison
perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 634650.
George, J. M. (2007). Creativity in organizations. The Academy of Management Annals, 1,
439477.
Gerhart, B., & Fang, M. (2014). Pay for (individual) performance: Issues, claims, evidence and the role
of sorting effects. Human Resource Management Review, 24, 41-52.
Gläser, D., van Gils, S., & Van Quaquebeke, N. (2017). Pay-for performance and interpersonal
deviance: Competitiveness as the match that lights the fire. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 16,
7891.
Gupta, N., & Shaw, J. D. (2014). Employee compensation: The neglected area of HRM research.
Human Resource Management Review, 24, 1-4.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2007). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in exercise
and sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Hardré, P. L., & Reeve, J. (2009). Training corporate managers to adopt a more autonomy-supportive
motivating style toward employees: An intervention study. International Journal of Training and
Development, 13, 165-184.
Hershcovis, M. S., & Barling, J. (2010). Towards a multifoci approach to workplace aggression: A
meta-analytic review of outcomes from different perpetrators. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
31, 2444.
Houlfort, N., Koestner, R., Joussemet, M., Nantel-Vivier, A., & Lekes, N. (2002). The impact of
performance-contingent rewards on perceived autonomy and competence. Motivation and
Emotion, 26, 279-295.
Johnson, D. A., Dickinson, A. M., & Huitema, B. E. (2008). The effects of objective feedback
when individuals receive fixed and individual incentive pay. Performance Improvement
Quarterly, 20, 5374.
Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Simon, L. S., & Rich, B. L. (2012). The psychic cost of doing wrong:
Ethical conflict, divestiture socialization, and emotional exhaustion. Journal of Management,
38, 784808.
Kasser, T. (2016). Materialistic values and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 489-514.
Kouchaki, M., Smith-Crowe, K., Brief, A. P., & Sousa, C. (2013). Seeing green: Mere exposure to
money triggers a business decision frame and unethical outcomes. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 121, 53-61.
Krug, M. K., & Braver, T. S. (2014). Motivation and cognitive control: Going beyond monetary
incentives. In E. Bijleveld, & H. Aarts (Eds.), The psychological science of money (pp. 137- 162).
New York, NY: Springer.
Int. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management / Business-and-Management.org
16
Kushlev, K., Dunn, E. W., & Lucas, R. E. (2015). Higher income is associated with less daily sadness
but not more daily happiness. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 483-489.
Kuvaas, B. (2006). Work performance, affective commitment, and work motivation: The roles of pay
administration and pay level. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 365-385.
Madhani, P. M. (2014). Compensation, ethical sales behavior and customer lifetime value.
Compensation & Benefits Review, 46, 204-218.
Moller, A. C., & Deci, E. L. (2014). The psychology of getting paid: An integrated perspective. In E.
Bijleveld, & H. Aarts (Eds.), The psychological science of money (pp. 137-162). New York:
Springer.
Olafsen, A. H., Halvari, H., Forest, J., & Deci, E. L. (2015). Show them the money? The role of pay,
managerial need support, and justice in a self-determination theory model of intrinsic work
motivation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 56, 447-457.
Ones, D. S. (2002). Introduction to the special issue on counterproductive behaviors at work.
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 10, 14.
Park, L. E., Ward, D. E., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2017). It’s all about the money (for some):
Consequences of financially contingent self-worth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
43, 601622.
Porath, C. L., & Pearson, C. M. (2010). The cost of bad behavior. Organizational Dynamics, 39, 6471.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects
in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of
Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437-448.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Self-determination theory and the role of basic psychological needs
in personality and the organization of behavior. In O. P. John, R. Robins, & L. A. Lawrence
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed.; pp. 654-678). New York: Guilford
Press.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2011). A self-determination theory perspective on social, institutional,
cultural, and economic supports for autonomy and their importance for well-being. In V. I.
Chirkov, R. Ryan, & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.), Human autonomy in cross-cultural context (pp. 45-
64). Netherlands: Springer.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation,
development, and wellness. New York, NY, US: Guilford Publications.
Thau, S., Aquino, K., & Poortvliet, P. M. (2007). Self-defeating behaviors inorganizations: The
relationship between thwarted belonging and interpersonal work behaviors. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 92, 840847.
Thibault Landry, A. T., Kindlein, J., Trépanier, S. G., Forest, J., Zigarmi, D., Houson, D., & Brodbeck,
F. C. (2016). Why individuals want money is what matters: Using self-determination theory to
explain the differential relationship between motives for making money and employee
psychological health. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 226-242.
Thibault, Landry., A., Gagné, M., Forest, J., Guerrero, S., Seguin, M., & Papachristopoulos, K.
(2017a). The relation between financial incentives, motivation, and performance: An integrative
SDT based investigation. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 16, 61-76.
Thibault Landry, A., Forest, J., Zigarmi, D., Houson, D., & Boucher, É. (2017b). The Carrot or the
stick? Investigating the functional meaning of cash rewards and their motivational power
according to self-determination theory. Compensation & Benefits, 49, 925.
Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Witte, H., Soenens, B., & Lens, W. (2010). Capturing
autonomy, competence, and relatedness at work: Construction and initial validation of the work-
related basic need satisfaction scale. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 83,
981-1002.
Vansteenkiste, M., Neyrinck, B., Niemiec, C. P., Soenens, B., Witte, H., & Broeck, A. (2007). On the
relations among work value orientations, psychological need satisfaction and job outcomes: A
self-determination theory approach. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80,
251-277.
Williams, G. C., Halvari, H., Niemiec, C. P., Sorebo, O., Olafsen, A. H., & Westbye, C. (2014).
Managerial support for basic psychological needs, somatic symptom burden and work-related
correlates: A self-determination theory perspective. Work & Stress, 28, 404-419.